New paper by Anna Laurinavichyute and me:
The (ir)reproducibility of published analyses: A case study of 57 JML articles published between 2019 and 2021
Download from: https://psyarxiv.com/hf297/
This blog is a repository of cool things relating to statistical computing, simulation and stochastic modeling.
New paper by Anna Laurinavichyute and me:
The (ir)reproducibility of published analyses: A case study of 57 JML articles published between 2019 and 2021
Download from: https://psyarxiv.com/hf297/
Applications are now open for the sixth SMLP summer school, to be held in person (hopefully) in the Griebnitzsee campus of the University of Potsdam, Germany, 12-16 Sept 2022.
Apply here: https://vasishth.github.io/smlp2022/
SAFAL 2: The Second South Asian Forum on the Acquisition and Processing of Language (30-31 August, 2021
Details: https://sites.google.com/view/safal2021/home
The first South Asian Forum on the Acquisition and Processing of Language (SAFAL) highlighted the need to provide a platform for showcasing and discussing acquisition and processing research in the context of South Asian languages. The second edition aims to build on this endeavour.
Following the first edition, the Second South Asian Forum on the Acquisition and Processing of Language (SAFAL) aims to provide a platform to exchange research on sentence processing, verbal semantics, computational modeling, corpus-based psycholinguistics, neurobiology of language, and child language acquisition, among others, in the context of the subcontinent's linguistic landscape.
Invited speakers:
Sakshi Bhatia is an Assistant Professor of Linguistics at the Central University of Rajasthan. Her research areas include syntax, psycholinguistics and the syntax-psycholinguistics interface
Kamal Choudhary is an Assistant Professor of Linguistics at the Indian Institute of Technology Ropar. His research areas include neurobiology of language and syntactic typology.
Subsetting data to carry out pairwise comparisons within one level of a 2-level or 3-level factor can lead to misleading results. This happens because by subsetting, we inadvertently drop variance components.
The data and code for both posts can be accessed from:
A very common mistake I see in psycholinguistics and psychology papers is subsetting the data to carry out an analysis.
In this post, I will discuss the second of two examples; the first appeared here.
It is worth repeating that in both examples, there is absolutely no implication that the authors did anything dishonest—they did their analyses in good faith. The broader problem is that in psychology and linguistics, we are rarely taught much about data analysis. We usually learn a canned cookbook style of analysis. I am pretty sure I have made this mistake in the past—I remember doing this kind of subsetting; this was before I had learnt how to do contrast coding.
I also want to point out what a big deal it is that the authors released their data. This usually happens in only about 25% of the cases, in my experience. This estimate is remarkably close to that from published estimates on the willingness of researchers to release data. So, kudos to these researchers for being among the top 25%.
So here is the second example. I originally analyzed this data to create some exercises for my students, but the theoretical questions addressed in this paper are inherently interesting for psycholinguists.
The paper we consider here is:
This paper has been cited some 267 times at the moment of writing this post, according to google scholar. It appears in one of our top journals, Journal of Memory and Language (JML), which apparently has an impact factor of 4.014 (I like the precision to three decimal places! :) according to google. I believe this is high. By comparison, Cognition seems to be only at 3.537.
The central claim that the paper makes is summarized nicely in the abstract; I highlight the relevant part of the abstract below:
This paper reports the results of a dual-task experiment which investigates the nature of working memory resources used in sentence comprehension. Participants read sentences of varying syntactic complexity (containing subject- and object-extracted relative clauses) while remembering one or three nouns (similar to or dissimilar from the sentence-nouns). A significant on-line interaction was found between syntactic complexity and similarity between the memory-nouns and the sentence-nouns in the three memory-nouns conditions, such that the similarity between the memory-nouns and the sentence-nouns affected the more complex object-extracted relative clauses to a greater extent than the less complex subject-extracted relative clauses. These results extend Gordon, Hendrick, and Levine’s (2002) report of a trend of such an interaction. The results argue against the domain-specific view of working memory resources in sentence comprehension (Caplan & Waters, 1999).
What I will show below is that there is no evidence for the claimed interaction that is highlighted above. The only solid result in this paper is that object relatives are harder to process than subject relatives. Obviously, this is actually interesting for psycholinguists; but it is not a newsworthy result and if that had been the main claim, JML would have rejected this paper (although Cognition seems to love papers showing that object relatives are easier or harder process than subject relatives ;).
I want to stress at the outset that although I show that there is no evidence for the claimed interaction in the abstract above, this does not mean there is no interaction. There might still be. We just don’t know either way from this work.
The data are from this paper:
Fedorenko, E., Gibson, E., & Rohde, D. (2006). The nature of working memory capacity in sentence comprehension: Evidence against domain-specific working memory resources. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(4), 541-553.
Ev was kind enough to give me the data; many thanks to her for this.
The study is a self-paced reading experiment, with a 2x2x2 design, the examples are as follows:
easy: Joel
hard: Joel-Greg-Andy
name: Joel-Greg-Andy
occ: poet-cartoonist-voter
As hinted above, this design is not exactly 2x2x2; there is also a within items manipulation of relative clause version. That detail was ignored in the published analysis, so we ignore it here as well.
The main claim of the paper is laid out in the abstract above, but I repeat it below:
“A significant on-line interaction was found between syntactic complexity and similarity between the memory-nouns and the sentence-nouns in the three memory-nouns conditions, such that the similarity between the memory-nouns and the sentence-nouns affected the more complex object-extracted relative clauses to a greater extent than the less complex subject-extracted relative clauses.”
The General Discussion explains this further. I highlight the important parts.
“The most interesting result presented here is an interaction between syntactic complexity and the memory- noun/sentence-noun similarity during the critical region of the linguistic materials in the hard-load (three memory-nouns) conditions: people processed object-extracted relative clauses more slowly when they had to maintain a set of nouns that were similar to the nouns in the sentence than when they had to maintain a set of nouns that were dissimilar from the nouns in the sentence; in contrast, for the less complex subject-extracted relative clauses, there was no reading time difference between the similar and dissimilar memory load conditions. In the easy-load (one memory-noun) conditions, no interaction between syntactic complexity and memory-noun/sentence-noun similarity was observed. These results provide evidence against the hypothesis whereby there is a pool of domain-specific verbal working memory resources for sentence comprehension, contra Caplan and Waters (1999). Specifically, the results reported here extend the off-line results reported by Gordon et al. (2002) who—using a similar dual-task paradigm—provided evidence of an interaction between syntactic complexity and memory-noun/sentence-noun similarity in response-accuracies to questions about the content of the sentences. Although there was also a suggestion of an on-line reading time interaction in Gordon et al.’s experiment, this effect did not reach significance. The effect of memory-noun type in subject-extracted conditions was 12.7 ms per word (50.8ms over the four-word relative clause region), and the effect of memory-noun type in object-extracted conditions was 40.1 ms (160.4 ms over the four-word relative clause region). (Thanks to Bill Levine for providing these means). In the hard-load conditions of our experiment, the effect of memory-noun type in subject-extracted conditions over the four-word relative clause region is 12 ms in raw RTs (18 ms in residual RTs), and the effect of memory- noun type in object-extracted conditions is 324 ms in raw RTs (388 ms in residual RTs). This difference in effect sizes of memory-noun type between Gordon et al.’s and our experiment is plausibly responsible for the interaction observed in the current experiment, and the lack of such an interaction in Gordon et al.’s study. The current results therefore demonstrate that Gordon et al.’s results extend to on-line processing.”
Table 3 in the paper (last line) reports the critical interaction claimed for the hard-load conditions (I added the p-values, which were not provided in the table):
“Synt x noun type [hard load]: F1(1,43)=6.58, p=0.014;F2(1,31)=6.79, p=0.014”.
There is also a minF’ provided: \(minF'(1,72)=3.34, p=0.07\). I also checked the minF’ using the standard formula from a classic paper by Herb Clark:
Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12(4), 335-359.
[Aside: The journal “Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior” is now called Journal of Memory and Language.]
Here is the minF’ function:
minf <- function(f1,f2,n1,n2){
fprime <- (f1*f2)/(f1+f2)
n <- round(((f1+f2)*(f1+f2))/(((f1*f1)/n2)+((f2*f2)/n1)))
pval<-pf(fprime,1,n,lower.tail=FALSE)
return(paste("minF(1,",n,")=",round(fprime,digits=2),"p-value=",round(pval,2),"crit=",round(qf(.95,1,n))))
}
And here is the p-value for the minF’ calculation:
minf(6.58,6.79,43,31)
## [1] "minF(1, 72 )= 3.34 p-value= 0.07 crit= 4"
The first thing to notice here is that even the published claim (about an RC type-noun type interaction in the hard load condition) is not statistically significant. I have heard through the grapevine that JML allows the authors to consider an effect as statistically significant even if the MinF’ is not significant. Apparently the reason is that the MinF’ calculation is considered to be conservative. But if they are going to ignore it, why the JML wants people to report a MinF’ is a mystery to me.
Declaring a non-significant result to be significant is a very common situation in the Journal of Memory and Language, and other journals. Some other JML papers I know of that report a non-significant minF’ as their main finding in the paper, but still act as if they have significance are:
Dillon et al 2013: “For total times this analysis revealed a three-way interaction that was significant by participants and marginal by items (\(F_1(1,39)=8.0,...,p=<0.01; F_1(1,47)=4.0,...,p=<0.055; minF'(1,81)=2,66,p=0.11\))”.
Van Dyke and McElree 2006: “The interaction of LoadxInterference was significant, both by subjects and by items. … minF’(1,90)=2.35, p = 0.13.”
Incidentally, neither of the main claims in these two studies replicated (which is not the same thing as saying that their claims are false—we don’t know whether they are false or true):
The second paper was rejected by JML, by the way.
## Download and install library from here:
## https://github.com/vasishth/lingpsych
library(lingpsych)
data("df_fedorenko06")
head(df_fedorenko06)
## rctype nountype load item subj region RT
## 2 obj name hard 16 1 2 4454
## 6 subj name hard 4 1 2 5718
## 10 subj name easy 10 1 2 2137
## 14 subj name hard 20 1 2 903
## 18 subj name hard 12 1 2 1837
## 22 subj occ hard 19 1 2 1128
As explained above, we have a 2x2x2 design: rctype [obj,subj] x nountype [name, occupation] x load [hard,easy]. Region 2 is the critical region, the entire relative clause, and RT is the reading time for this region. Subject and item columns are self-explanatory.
We notice immediately that something is wrong in this data frame. Compare subject 1 vs the other subjects:
## something is wrong here:
xtabs(~subj+item,df_fedorenko06)
## item
## subj 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28
## 1 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2
## 2 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 3 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 4 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 5 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 6 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 7 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 8 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 9 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 11 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 12 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 13 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 14 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 15 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 16 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 17 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 18 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 19 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 20 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 21 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 22 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 23 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 24 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 25 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 26 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 28 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 29 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 30 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 31 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 32 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 33 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 34 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 35 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 36 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 37 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 38 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 39 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 40 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 41 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 42 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 43 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 44 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 46 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## 47 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
## item
## subj 29 30 31 32
## 1 2 2 2 2
## 2 1 1 1 1
## 3 1 1 1 1
## 4 1 1 1 1
## 5 1 1 1 1
## 6 1 1 1 1
## 7 1 1 1 1
## 8 1 1 1 1
## 9 1 1 1 1
## 11 1 1 1 1
## 12 1 1 1 1
## 13 1 1 1 1
## 14 1 1 1 1
## 15 1 1 1 1
## 16 1 1 1 1
## 17 1 1 1 1
## 18 1 1 1 1
## 19 1 1 1 1
## 20 1 1 1 1
## 21 1 1 1 1
## 22 1 1 1 1
## 23 1 1 1 1
## 24 1 1 1 1
## 25 1 1 1 1
## 26 1 1 1 1
## 28 1 1 1 1
## 29 1 1 1 1
## 30 1 1 1 1
## 31 1 1 1 1
## 32 1 1 1 1
## 33 1 1 1 1
## 34 1 1 1 1
## 35 1 1 1 1
## 36 1 1 1 1
## 37 1 1 1 1
## 38 1 1 1 1
## 39 1 1 1 1
## 40 1 1 1 1
## 41 1 1 1 1
## 42 1 1 1 1
## 43 1 1 1 1
## 44 1 1 1 1
## 46 1 1 1 1
## 47 1 1 1 1
Subject 1 saw all items twice! There was apparently a bug in the code, or maybe two different subjects were mis-labeled as subject 1. Hard to know. This is one more reason why one should not load and analyze data immediately; always check that the design was correctly implemented.
We could remove subject 1 from this data, leaving us with 43 subjects:
df_fedorenko06<-subset(df_fedorenko06,subj!=1)
sort(unique(df_fedorenko06$subj))
length(unique(df_fedorenko06$subj))
But we won’t do this yet.
First, set up an ANOVA style coding for the data, in lmer:
library(lme4)
## Loading required package: Matrix
df_fedorenko06$noun<-ifelse(df_fedorenko06$nountype=="name",1/2,-1/2)
df_fedorenko06$ld<-ifelse(df_fedorenko06$load=="hard",1/2,-1/2)
df_fedorenko06$rc<-ifelse(df_fedorenko06$rctype=="obj",1/2,-1/2)
df_fedorenko06$nounxld<-df_fedorenko06$noun*df_fedorenko06$ld*2
df_fedorenko06$nounxrc<-df_fedorenko06$noun*df_fedorenko06$rc*2
df_fedorenko06$ldxrc<-df_fedorenko06$ld*df_fedorenko06$rc*2
df_fedorenko06$nounxldxrc<-df_fedorenko06$noun*df_fedorenko06$ld*df_fedorenko06$rc*4
I am using effect coding here (\(\pm 1/2\)), so that every estimate reflects the estimated difference in means for the particular comparison being made. When multiplying to get an interaction, I have to multiply with a factor of 2 or 4 to get the effect coding back; if I don’t do that I will get \(\pm 1/4\) or \(\pm 1/8\).
I’m using effect coding because I was interested in getting an estimate of the relative clause effect for our Bayesian textbook’s chapter 6, on prior self-elicitation.
The data frame now has new columns representing the contrast coding:
head(df_fedorenko06)
## rctype nountype load item subj region RT noun ld rc nounxld nounxrc
## 2 obj name hard 16 1 2 4454 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5
## 6 subj name hard 4 1 2 5718 0.5 0.5 -0.5 0.5 -0.5
## 10 subj name easy 10 1 2 2137 0.5 -0.5 -0.5 -0.5 -0.5
## 14 subj name hard 20 1 2 903 0.5 0.5 -0.5 0.5 -0.5
## 18 subj name hard 12 1 2 1837 0.5 0.5 -0.5 0.5 -0.5
## 22 subj occ hard 19 1 2 1128 -0.5 0.5 -0.5 -0.5 0.5
## ldxrc nounxldxrc
## 2 0.5 0.5
## 6 -0.5 -0.5
## 10 0.5 0.5
## 14 -0.5 -0.5
## 18 -0.5 -0.5
## 22 -0.5 0.5
Display the estimated means in each condition, just to get some idea of what’s coming.
round(with(df_fedorenko06,tapply(RT,nountype,mean)))
## name occ
## 1636 1754
round(with(df_fedorenko06,tapply(RT,load,mean)))
## easy hard
## 1608 1782
round(with(df_fedorenko06,tapply(RT,rctype,mean)))
## obj subj
## 1909 1482
The authors report raw and residual RTs, but we analyze raw RTs, and then analyze log RTs. There’s actually no point in computing residual RTs (I will discuss this at some point in a different blog post), so we ignore the residuals analysis, although nothing much will change in my main points below even if we were to use residual RTs.
I fit the most complex model I could. If I were a Bayesian (which I am), I would have done the full Monty just because I can, but never mind.
## raw RTs
m1<-lmer(RT~noun+ld+rc+nounxld + nounxrc + ldxrc + nounxldxrc+(1+ld+rc+ nounxrc + nounxldxrc||subj) + (1+ld+rc+ nounxrc + nounxldxrc||item),df_fedorenko06)
#summary(m1)
## log RTs
m2<-lmer(log(RT)~noun+ld+rc+nounxld + nounxrc + ldxrc + nounxldxrc+(1+ld+ nounxrc||subj) + (1+ld+rc||item),df_fedorenko06)
#summary(m2)
Here is the summary output from the two models:
## raw RTs:
summary(m1)$coefficients
## Estimate Std. Error t value
## (Intercept) 1675.79757 125.88880 13.3117286
## noun -116.07053 58.60273 -1.9806337
## ld 155.14812 112.39971 1.3803249
## rc 422.73102 77.84215 5.4306182
## nounxld -103.39623 58.60364 -1.7643312
## nounxrc -110.87780 61.73703 -1.7959692
## ldxrc -23.47607 58.62151 -0.4004685
## nounxldxrc -95.76489 66.95281 -1.4303340
## log RTs:
summary(m2)$coefficients
## Estimate Std. Error t value
## (Intercept) 7.20041271 0.06585075 109.3444271
## noun -0.03127831 0.02457298 -1.2728741
## ld 0.01532113 0.04530999 0.3381403
## rc 0.21828914 0.02812620 7.7610591
## nounxld -0.04645901 0.02457339 -1.8906222
## nounxrc -0.02132791 0.02564043 -0.8318078
## ldxrc -0.03704690 0.02457244 -1.5076607
## nounxldxrc -0.01944255 0.02462971 -0.7893945
Both the models only show that object relatives are harder to process than subject relatives. No other main effect or interactions pan out. This (non-)result is actually consistent with what’s reported in the paper.
For the story about the hard load conditions (discussed below) to hold up, a prerequisite is that the nounxldxrc interaction be significant, but it is not.
The Syn x noun interaction in the hard load conditions reported in the paper as the main result was computed by first subsetting the data such that we have only the hard load conditions’ data. This leads to a 2x2 design. Let’s see what happens when we do this analysis by subsetting the data. We have dropped half the data:
dim(df_fedorenko06)
## [1] 1440 14
fed06hard<-subset(df_fedorenko06,load=="hard")
dim(fed06hard)
## [1] 720 14
(1440-720)/1440
## [1] 0.5
Let’s first do a repeated measures ANOVA (=paired t-tests) on raw RTs by subject and by item, because that is what Fedorenko et al did (well, they used residual RTs, but nothing will change if we use that dependent measure).
## aggregate by subject:
fedsubjRC<-aggregate(RT~subj+rctype,mean,data=fed06hard)
head(fedsubjRC)
## subj rctype RT
## 1 1 obj 3387.500
## 2 2 obj 892.125
## 3 3 obj 1373.000
## 4 4 obj 1738.625
## 5 5 obj 7074.000
## 6 6 obj 3119.625
t.test(RT~rctype,paired=TRUE,fedsubjRC)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: RT by rctype
## t = 3.5883, df = 43, p-value = 0.0008469
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## 176.7331 630.3151
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## 403.5241
fedsubjN<-aggregate(RT~subj+nountype,mean,data=fed06hard)
head(fedsubjN)
## subj nountype RT
## 1 1 name 2971.500
## 2 2 name 928.875
## 3 3 name 1161.500
## 4 4 name 1414.125
## 5 5 name 4472.250
## 6 6 name 1831.875
t.test(RT~nountype,paired=TRUE,fedsubjN)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: RT by nountype
## t = -2.6432, df = 43, p-value = 0.01141
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## -383.91794 -51.61331
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## -217.7656
## interaction: diff between obj and subj in name vs in occ conditions:
RCocc<-aggregate(RT~subj+rctype,mean,data=subset(fed06hard,nountype=="occ"))
diff_occ<-subset(RCocc,rctype=="obj")$RT-subset(RCocc,rctype=="subj")$RT
RCname<-aggregate(RT~subj+rctype,mean,data=subset(fed06hard,nountype=="name"))
diff_name<-subset(RCname,rctype=="obj")$RT-subset(RCname,rctype=="subj")$RT
## by-subject interaction:
t.test(diff_occ,diff_name,paired=TRUE)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: diff_occ and diff_name
## t = 2.131, df = 43, p-value = 0.03885
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## 21.71398 787.90534
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## 404.8097
One can of course do this with an ANOVA function:
fedsubjANOVA<-aggregate(RT~subj+rctype+nountype,mean,data=fed06hard)
head(fedsubjANOVA)
## subj rctype nountype RT
## 1 1 obj name 3030.25
## 2 2 obj name 962.50
## 3 3 obj name 1087.50
## 4 4 obj name 1687.00
## 5 5 obj name 5006.75
## 6 6 obj name 1064.50
library(rstatix)
##
## Attaching package: 'rstatix'
## The following object is masked from 'package:stats':
##
## filter
subj_anova_hard<-anova_test(data = fedsubjANOVA,
dv = RT,
wid = subj,
within = c(rctype,nountype)
)
get_anova_table(subj_anova_hard)
## ANOVA Table (type III tests)
##
## Effect DFn DFd F p p<.05 ges
## 1 rctype 1 43 12.876 0.000847 * 0.031
## 2 nountype 1 43 6.986 0.011000 * 0.009
## 3 rctype:nountype 1 43 4.541 0.039000 * 0.008
We see a significant interaction by subjects, as reported in the paper.
By items:
## aggregate by item:
feditemRC<-aggregate(RT~item+rctype,mean,data=fed06hard)
head(feditemRC)
## item rctype RT
## 1 1 obj 1978.545
## 2 2 obj 2076.667
## 3 3 obj 3483.273
## 4 4 obj 2489.000
## 5 5 obj 1322.091
## 6 6 obj 1268.800
t.test(RT~rctype,paired=TRUE,feditemRC)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: RT by rctype
## t = 3.9827, df = 31, p-value = 0.0003833
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## 194.5260 602.8663
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## 398.6962
feditemN<-aggregate(RT~item+nountype,mean,data=fed06hard)
head(feditemN)
## item nountype RT
## 1 1 name 1573.727
## 2 2 name 1566.455
## 3 3 name 1580.364
## 4 4 name 2808.667
## 5 5 name 1251.000
## 6 6 name 1261.818
t.test(RT~nountype,paired=TRUE,feditemN)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: RT by nountype
## t = -1.6978, df = 31, p-value = 0.09957
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## -477.39928 43.65591
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## -216.8717
## interaction: diff between obj and item in name vs in occ conditions:
RCocc<-aggregate(RT~item+rctype,mean,data=subset(fed06hard,nountype=="occ"))
diff_occ<-subset(RCocc,rctype=="obj")$RT-subset(RCocc,rctype=="subj")$RT
RCname<-aggregate(RT~item+rctype,mean,data=subset(fed06hard,nountype=="name"))
diff_name<-subset(RCname,rctype=="obj")$RT-subset(RCname,rctype=="subj")$RT
## by-item interaction:
t.test(diff_occ,diff_name,paired=TRUE)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: diff_occ and diff_name
## t = 2.0274, df = 31, p-value = 0.05129
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## -2.45258 823.20437
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## 410.3759
Using ANOVA:
feditemANOVA<-aggregate(RT~item+rctype+nountype,mean,data=fed06hard)
head(feditemANOVA)
## item rctype nountype RT
## 1 1 obj name 2206.000
## 2 2 obj name 1946.667
## 3 3 obj name 1966.167
## 4 4 obj name 2910.600
## 5 5 obj name 1011.500
## 6 6 obj name 1496.200
item_anova_hard<-anova_test(data = feditemANOVA,
dv = RT,
wid = item,
within = c(rctype,nountype)
)
get_anova_table(item_anova_hard)
## ANOVA Table (type III tests)
##
## Effect DFn DFd F p p<.05 ges
## 1 rctype 1 31 14.802 0.000557 * 0.085
## 2 nountype 1 31 2.867 0.100000 0.028
## 3 rctype:nountype 1 31 4.110 0.051000 0.023
Notice that even if the by-subjects and by-items analyses are significant, the MinF’ is not. As Clark points out in his paper, MinF’ is critical to compute in order to draw inferences about the hypothesis test.
Since \(F=t^2\), we can compute the MinF’.
F1<-2.13^2
F2<-2.03^2
minf(F1,F2,43,31)
## [1] "minF(1, 71 )= 2.16 p-value= 0.15 crit= 4"
We can do this MinF’ calculation with the ANOVA output as well:
minf(4.541,4.11,43,31)
## [1] "minF(1, 71 )= 2.16 p-value= 0.15 crit= 4"
We get exactly the same MinF’ in both the t-tests and ANOVA because they are doing the same test.
The higher-order interaction is not significant. This diverges from the main claim in the paper.
Now, let’s do the same by-subjects and items analyses using logRTs:
fed06hard$logRT<-log(fed06hard$RT)
## aggregate by subject:
fedsubjRC<-aggregate(logRT~subj+rctype,mean,data=fed06hard)
head(fedsubjRC)
## subj rctype logRT
## 1 1 obj 7.980754
## 2 2 obj 6.710152
## 3 3 obj 7.125770
## 4 4 obj 7.179976
## 5 5 obj 8.625057
## 6 6 obj 7.569662
t.test(logRT~rctype,paired=TRUE,fedsubjRC)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: logRT by rctype
## t = 4.6431, df = 43, p-value = 3.227e-05
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## 0.1024094 0.2596785
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## 0.1810439
fedsubjN<-aggregate(logRT~subj+nountype,mean,data=fed06hard)
head(fedsubjN)
## subj nountype logRT
## 1 1 name 7.835673
## 2 2 name 6.740148
## 3 3 name 6.988723
## 4 4 name 7.167295
## 5 5 name 8.249557
## 6 6 name 7.142238
t.test(logRT~nountype,paired=TRUE,fedsubjN)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: logRT by nountype
## t = -2.2556, df = 43, p-value = 0.02924
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## -0.151468895 -0.008471231
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## -0.07997006
## interaction: diff between obj and subj in name vs in occ conditions:
RCocc<-aggregate(logRT~subj+rctype,mean,data=subset(fed06hard,nountype=="occ"))
diff_occ<-subset(RCocc,rctype=="obj")$logRT-subset(RCocc,rctype=="subj")$logRT
RCname<-aggregate(logRT~subj+rctype,mean,data=subset(fed06hard,nountype=="name"))
diff_name<-subset(RCname,rctype=="obj")$logRT-subset(RCname,rctype=="subj")$logRT
## by-subject interaction:
t.test(diff_occ,diff_name,paired=TRUE)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: diff_occ and diff_name
## t = 1.0226, df = 43, p-value = 0.3122
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## -0.07614228 0.23279035
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## 0.07832404
Now the by-subjects t-test for the interaction is nowhere near significant (cf the raw RTs analysis above). This means that there were some extreme values in the by-subjects data driving the effect.
By items:
## aggregate by item:
feditemRC<-aggregate(logRT~item+rctype,mean,data=fed06hard)
head(feditemRC)
## item rctype logRT
## 1 1 obj 7.421371
## 2 2 obj 7.339790
## 3 3 obj 7.562565
## 4 4 obj 7.590270
## 5 5 obj 7.016257
## 6 6 obj 7.031505
t.test(logRT~rctype,paired=TRUE,feditemRC)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: logRT by rctype
## t = 3.6988, df = 31, p-value = 0.0008374
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## 0.08105886 0.28033104
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## 0.180695
feditemN<-aggregate(logRT~item+nountype,mean,data=fed06hard)
head(feditemN)
## item nountype logRT
## 1 1 name 7.216759
## 2 2 name 6.991090
## 3 3 name 7.192081
## 4 4 name 7.707697
## 5 5 name 7.018699
## 6 6 name 6.981104
t.test(logRT~nountype,paired=TRUE,feditemN)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: logRT by nountype
## t = -1.4239, df = 31, p-value = 0.1645
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## -0.18854611 0.03351487
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## -0.07751562
## interaction: diff between obj and item in name vs in occ conditions:
RCocc<-aggregate(logRT~item+rctype,mean,data=subset(fed06hard,nountype=="occ"))
diff_occ<-subset(RCocc,rctype=="obj")$logRT-subset(RCocc,rctype=="subj")$logRT
RCname<-aggregate(logRT~item+rctype,mean,data=subset(fed06hard,nountype=="name"))
diff_name<-subset(RCname,rctype=="obj")$logRT-subset(RCname,rctype=="subj")$logRT
## by-item interaction:
t.test(diff_occ,diff_name,paired=TRUE)
##
## Paired t-test
##
## data: diff_occ and diff_name
## t = 0.93122, df = 31, p-value = 0.3589
## alternative hypothesis: true difference in means is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## -0.09504043 0.25475215
## sample estimates:
## mean of the differences
## 0.07985586
Here, too, the interaction by items is not significant.
Conclusion: the MinF’ values show that there is no evidence for an interaction in the hard conditions, regardless of whether we use raw or log RTs.
But there is actually more to this story, as I show below with linear mixed models analyses.
Now, we do the same analyses as above, but in lmer instead of paired t-tests, and by subsetting the data as Fedorenko did.
In order to compare this model (m4) with the correct analysis (model m6 below), we change the coding to be \(\pm 1\) instead of \(\pm 1/2\).
## 2x2 anova by doing subsetting: this is the main result in the paper
fed06hard<-subset(df_fedorenko06,load=="hard")
fed06hard$noun<-fed06hard$noun*2
fed06hard$rc<-fed06hard$rc*2
fed06hard$nounxrc<-fed06hard$nounxrc*2
fed06hard<-fed06hard[,c("subj","item","RT","noun","rc","nounxrc")]
head(fed06hard)
## subj item RT noun rc nounxrc
## 2 1 16 4454 1 1 1
## 6 1 4 5718 1 -1 -1
## 14 1 20 903 1 -1 -1
## 18 1 12 1837 1 -1 -1
## 22 1 19 1128 -1 -1 1
## 26 1 31 2742 -1 1 -1
Here is the analysis using subsetting (this is incorrect):
m4<-lmer(RT~noun+rc+nounxrc+(1+rc+nounxrc||subj) + (1+noun+nounxrc||item),fed06hard)
summary(m4)
## Linear mixed model fit by REML ['lmerMod']
## Formula: RT ~ noun + rc + nounxrc + ((1 | subj) + (0 + rc | subj) + (0 +
## nounxrc | subj)) + ((1 | item) + (0 + noun | item) + (0 +
## nounxrc | item))
## Data: fed06hard
##
## REML criterion at convergence: 12401.9
##
## Scaled residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -3.0317 -0.4256 -0.1341 0.2131 11.6983
##
## Random effects:
## Groups Name Variance Std.Dev.
## subj (Intercept) 915329 956.73
## subj.1 rc 42511 206.18
## subj.2 nounxrc 1570 39.62
## item (Intercept) 56296 237.27
## item.1 noun 4123 64.21
## item.2 nounxrc 11841 108.82
## Residual 1540203 1241.05
## Number of obs: 720, groups: subj, 44; item, 32
##
## Fixed effects:
## Estimate Std. Error t value
## (Intercept) 1752.09 157.25 11.142
## noun -110.39 47.65 -2.317
## rc 200.80 55.86 3.595
## nounxrc -103.48 50.56 -2.047
##
## Correlation of Fixed Effects:
## (Intr) noun rc
## noun 0.000
## rc 0.000 0.000
## nounxrc 0.000 0.000 0.000
summary(m4)$coefficients
## Estimate Std. Error t value
## (Intercept) 1752.0945 157.24556 11.142410
## noun -110.3885 47.64559 -2.316866
## rc 200.8043 55.85609 3.595031
## nounxrc -103.4786 50.56305 -2.046527
The above analysis shows a significant interaction. But this analysis is incorrect because it’s dropping half the data, which contributes to misestimating the variance components.
Next, we will do the above analysis using all the data (no subsetting to load==hard conditions). We do this by defining nested contrasts
The design is:
## Nested analysis:
## load e e e e h h h h
## noun o o n n o o n n
## rctyp s o s o s o s o
# a b c d e f g h
#---------------------------------------
#load -1 -1 -1 -1 1 1 1 1
#nn_easy -1 -1 1 1 0 0 0 0
#rc_easy -1 1 -1 1 0 0 0 0
#nn_hard 0 0 0 0 -1 -1 1 1
#rc_hard 0 0 0 0 -1 1 -1 1
Define the nested coding. This time I use \(\pm 1\) coding; this is just in order to facilitate quick model specification by multiplying main effects in the lmer equation.
I could have used \(\pm 1/2\) and the only thing that would give me is that the estimated coefficients would reflect the difference in means, but I would need to define interactions as separate columsn (as done above). The statistical results will not change as a function of these two coding schemes.
df_fedorenko06$ld<-ifelse(df_fedorenko06$load=="hard",1,-1)
df_fedorenko06$nn_easy<-ifelse(df_fedorenko06$load=="easy" & df_fedorenko06$nountype=="occ",-1,
ifelse(df_fedorenko06$load=="easy" & df_fedorenko06$nountype=="name",1,0))
df_fedorenko06$nn_hard<-ifelse(df_fedorenko06$load=="hard" & df_fedorenko06$nountype=="occ",-1,
ifelse(df_fedorenko06$load=="hard" & df_fedorenko06$nountype=="name",1,0))
df_fedorenko06$rc_easy<-ifelse(df_fedorenko06$load=="easy" & df_fedorenko06$rctype=="subj",-1,
ifelse(df_fedorenko06$load=="easy" & df_fedorenko06$rctype=="obj",1,0))
df_fedorenko06$rc_hard<-ifelse(df_fedorenko06$load=="hard" & df_fedorenko06$rctype=="subj",-1,
ifelse(df_fedorenko06$load=="hard" & df_fedorenko06$rctype=="obj",1,0))
m6<-lmer(RT~ld + nn_hard*rc_hard + nn_easy*rc_easy +
(1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard ||subj) +
(1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard + nn_easy||item),df_fedorenko06)
summary(m6)
## Linear mixed model fit by REML ['lmerMod']
## Formula: RT ~ ld + nn_hard * rc_hard + nn_easy * rc_easy + ((1 | subj) +
## (0 + ld | subj) + (0 + nn_hard:rc_hard | subj)) + ((1 | item) +
## (0 + ld | item) + (0 + nn_easy | item) + (0 + nn_hard:rc_hard | item))
## Data: df_fedorenko06
##
## REML criterion at convergence: 24455.1
##
## Scaled residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -2.7050 -0.4408 -0.1223 0.2148 13.3538
##
## Random effects:
## Groups Name Variance Std.Dev.
## subj (Intercept) 611484 781.97
## subj.1 ld 79352 281.70
## subj.2 nn_hard:rc_hard 20333 142.59
## item (Intercept) 36175 190.20
## item.1 ld 16443 128.23
## item.2 nn_easy 1915 43.76
## item.3 nn_hard:rc_hard 25223 158.82
## Residual 1252751 1119.26
## Number of obs: 1440, groups: subj, 44; item, 32
##
## Fixed effects:
## Estimate Std. Error t value
## (Intercept) 1675.796 126.128 13.286
## ld 77.641 56.535 1.373
## nn_hard -110.046 41.745 -2.636
## rc_hard 200.372 41.749 4.799
## nn_easy -6.734 42.434 -0.159
## rc_easy 224.046 41.749 5.366
## nn_hard:rc_hard -103.208 54.818 -1.883
## nn_easy:rc_easy -8.443 41.821 -0.202
##
## Correlation of Fixed Effects:
## (Intr) ld nn_hrd rc_hrd nn_esy rc_esy nn_h:_
## ld 0.000
## nn_hard 0.000 0.000
## rc_hard 0.000 0.000 0.000
## nn_easy 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000
## rc_easy 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000
## nn_hrd:rc_h 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000
## nn_sy:rc_sy 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.000
#qqPlot(residuals(m6))
#summary(m6)$coefficients
## compare with a null model, without the
## nn_hard:rc_hard interaction:
m6null<-lmer(RT~ld + nn_hard+rc_hard + nn_easy*rc_easy +(1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard ||subj) + (1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard + nn_easy||item),df_fedorenko06)
anova(m6,m6null)
## refitting model(s) with ML (instead of REML)
## Data: df_fedorenko06
## Models:
## m6null: RT ~ ld + nn_hard + rc_hard + nn_easy * rc_easy + ((1 | subj) + (0 + ld | subj) + (0 + nn_hard:rc_hard | subj)) + ((1 | item) + (0 + ld | item) + (0 + nn_easy | item) + (0 + nn_hard:rc_hard | item))
## m6: RT ~ ld + nn_hard * rc_hard + nn_easy * rc_easy + ((1 | subj) + (0 + ld | subj) + (0 + nn_hard:rc_hard | subj)) + ((1 | item) + (0 + ld | item) + (0 + nn_easy | item) + (0 + nn_hard:rc_hard | item))
## npar AIC BIC logLik deviance Chisq Df Pr(>Chisq)
## m6null 15 24566 24645 -12268 24536
## m6 16 24565 24649 -12266 24533 3.4482 1 0.06332 .
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
Removing subject 1 weakens the already non-significant interaction:
m6a<-lmer(RT~ld + nn_hard*rc_hard + nn_easy*rc_easy +(1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard ||subj) + (1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard + nn_easy||item),subset(df_fedorenko06,subj!=1))
#summary(m6a)
#qqPlot(residuals(m6))
#summary(m6)$coefficients
## compare with a null model, without the
## nn_hard:rc_hard interaction:
m6anull<-lmer(RT~ld + nn_hard+rc_hard + nn_easy*rc_easy +(1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard ||subj) + (1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard + nn_easy||item),subset(df_fedorenko06,subj!=1))
anova(m6a,m6anull)
## refitting model(s) with ML (instead of REML)
## Data: subset(df_fedorenko06, subj != 1)
## Models:
## m6anull: RT ~ ld + nn_hard + rc_hard + nn_easy * rc_easy + ((1 | subj) + (0 + ld | subj) + (0 + nn_hard:rc_hard | subj)) + ((1 | item) + (0 + ld | item) + (0 + nn_easy | item) + (0 + nn_hard:rc_hard | item))
## m6a: RT ~ ld + nn_hard * rc_hard + nn_easy * rc_easy + ((1 | subj) + (0 + ld | subj) + (0 + nn_hard:rc_hard | subj)) + ((1 | item) + (0 + ld | item) + (0 + nn_easy | item) + (0 + nn_hard:rc_hard | item))
## npar AIC BIC logLik deviance Chisq Df Pr(>Chisq)
## m6anull 15 23385 23463 -11677 23355
## m6a 16 23384 23467 -11676 23352 3.0927 1 0.07864 .
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
Notice that the interaction (nn_hard:rc_hard) is gone once we take the full data into account.
## subsetted data:
summary(m4)$coefficients[4,]
## Estimate Std. Error t value
## -103.478646 50.563049 -2.046527
## full data:
summary(m6)$coefficients[7,]
## Estimate Std. Error t value
## -103.208049 54.817661 -1.882752
What has changed is the standard error of the interaction (in m4 it is 51 ms, in m6 it is 55 ms); the increase in the SE is due to the increased number of variance components in the full model with the nested contrast coding. The subsetted analysis (m4, and the paired t-tests) are hiding important sources of variance and giving a misleading result.
Here are the variance components in model m4 (the subsetted data):
Random effects:
Groups Name Variance Std.Dev.
subj (Intercept) 915329 956.73
subj.1 rc 42511 206.18
subj.2 nounxrc 1570 39.62
item (Intercept) 56296 237.27
item.1 noun 4123 64.21
item.2 nounxrc 11841 108.82
Residual 1540203 1241.05
Here are the variance components in the full model:
Random effects:
Groups Name Variance Std.Dev.
subj (Intercept) 611472 781.97
subj.1 ld 317412 563.39
subj.2 nn_hard:rc_hard 20321 142.55
item (Intercept) 36172 190.19
item.1 ld 65756 256.43
item.2 nn_easy 1912 43.73
item.3 nn_hard:rc_hard 25229 158.84
Residual 1252759 1119.27
Notice that there are still some problems in these models:
library(car)
## Loading required package: carData
qqPlot(residuals(m6))
## 2826 546
## 707 137
boxplot(RT~rctype,df_fedorenko06)
What if we limit reading time to 10 seconds? What percent of the data would we lose? It turns out, very little, 0.3%:
nfull<-dim(df_fedorenko06)[1]
nfull
## [1] 1440
n10000<-dim(subset(df_fedorenko06,RT<10000))[1]
100*(nfull-n10000)/nfull
## [1] 0.2777778
m7<-lmer(RT~ld + nn_hard*rc_hard + nn_easy*rc_easy +(1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard ||subj) + (1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard + nn_easy||item),subset(df_fedorenko06,RT<10000))
## boundary (singular) fit: see ?isSingular
summary(m7)$coefficients
## Estimate Std. Error t value
## (Intercept) 1641.543453 116.01909 14.1489087
## ld 75.945797 47.41424 1.6017508
## nn_hard -75.395584 33.93436 -2.2218064
## rc_hard 165.834591 33.92088 4.8888646
## nn_easy 6.713027 33.89550 0.1980507
## rc_easy 192.150808 33.91198 5.6661624
## nn_hard:rc_hard -67.536307 51.79075 -1.3040226
## nn_easy:rc_easy 5.684016 33.98754 0.1672382
Notice that the interaction has shrunk from -102 ms (SE 50) in model m6 to -68 (52) in m7. This means there were a very small number of data points (0.3%) that were influential and leading to a much larger estimate.
Visualize these extreme values:
boxplot(RT~rctype+nountype,df_fedorenko06)
Plot the data without 0.3% of the extreme data:
boxplot(RT~rctype+nountype,subset(df_fedorenko06,RT<10000))
On the log ms scale, the extreme values are down-weighted:
boxplot(log(RT)~rctype+nountype,df_fedorenko06)
This means we can analyze the log ms data without deleting any extreme values:
m8<-lmer(log(RT)~ld + nn_hard*rc_hard + nn_easy*rc_easy +(1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard ||subj) + (1+ld + nn_hard:rc_hard||item),df_fedorenko06)
summary(m8)$coefficients
qqPlot(residuals(m8))
Basically, there is no hint of an interaction between noun type and RC in the hard conditions, contrary to the claim in the paper. This was the only finding in the paper, and it is not supported by the data. The key issue was subsetting the data, which inadvertently conceals important sources of variances.
If you subset the data to analyze effects within one level of a two- or three-level factor, you will usually get misleading results. The reason: by subsetting data, you are artificially reducing/misestimating the different sources of variance.
The scientific consequence of this subsetting error is that we have now drawn a misleading conclusion—we think we have evidence for an effect of working memory load on sentence processing difficulty. In fact, the data could be seen as consistent with exactly the opposite claim, that by Caplan and Waters, 1999. Does this mean that the claim in the Fedorenko et al paper was wrong? No! The claim may well be true. We just don’t know from these data.
Here, we demonstrate what happens to SE if we subset data as described above. The steps I take are:
library(MASS)
##
## Attaching package: 'MASS'
## The following object is masked from 'package:rstatix':
##
## select
## assumes that no. of subjects and no. of items is divisible by 8.
gen_fake_lnorm2x2x2<-function(ncond=8,
nitem=NULL,
nsubj=NULL,
beta=NULL,
Sigma_u=NULL, # subject vcov matrix
Sigma_w=NULL, # item vcov matrix
sigma_e=NULL){
grouping<-matrix(rep(NA,ncond*ncond),ncol=ncond)
grouping[1,]<-1:8
grouping[2,]<-c(2:8,1)
grouping[3,]<-c(3:8,1:2)
grouping[4,]<-c(4:8,1:3)
grouping[5,]<-c(5:8,1:4)
grouping[6,]<-c(6:8,1:5)
grouping[7,]<-c(7:8,1:6)
grouping[8,]<-c(8,1:7)
## prepare data frame for 8 condition latin square:
g1<-data.frame(item=1:nitem,
cond=rep(grouping[1,],nitem/ncond))
g2<-data.frame(item=1:nitem,
cond=rep(grouping[2,],nitem/ncond))
g3<-data.frame(item=1:nitem,
cond=rep(grouping[3,],nitem/ncond))
g4<-data.frame(item=1:nitem,
cond=rep(grouping[4,],nitem/ncond))
g5<-data.frame(item=1:nitem,
cond=rep(grouping[5,],nitem/ncond))
g6<-data.frame(item=1:nitem,
cond=rep(grouping[6,],nitem/ncond))
g7<-data.frame(item=1:nitem,
cond=rep(grouping[7,],nitem/ncond))
g8<-data.frame(item=1:nitem,
cond=rep(grouping[8,],nitem/ncond))
## assemble data frame:
gp1<-g1[rep(seq_len(nrow(g1)),
nsubj/ncond),]
gp2<-g2[rep(seq_len(nrow(g2)),
nsubj/ncond),]
gp3<-g3[rep(seq_len(nrow(g3)),
nsubj/ncond),]
gp4<-g4[rep(seq_len(nrow(g4)),
nsubj/ncond),]
gp5<-g5[rep(seq_len(nrow(g5)),
nsubj/ncond),]
gp6<-g6[rep(seq_len(nrow(g6)),
nsubj/ncond),]
gp7<-g7[rep(seq_len(nrow(g7)),
nsubj/ncond),]
gp8<-g8[rep(seq_len(nrow(g8)),
nsubj/ncond),]
fakedat<-rbind(gp1,gp2,gp3,gp4,gp5,gp6,gp7,gp8)
## add subjects:
fakedat$subj<-rep(1:nsubj,each=nitem)
## name conditions:
## Nested analysis:
# Cond: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
## load e e e e h h h h
## noun o o n n o o n n
## rctyp s o s o s o s o
fakedat$load<-ifelse(fakedat$cond%in%c(1,2,3,4),"easy","hard")
fakedat$nountype<-ifelse(fakedat$cond%in%c(1,2,5,6),"occ","name")
fakedat$rctype<-ifelse(fakedat$cond%in%c(1,3,5,7),"subj","obj")
## nested contrast coding:
fakedat$ld<-ifelse(fakedat$load=="easy",1,-1)
fakedat$nn_easy<-ifelse(fakedat$load=="easy" & fakedat$nountype=="occ",-1,
ifelse(fakedat$load=="easy" & fakedat$nountype=="name",1,0))
fakedat$nn_hard<-ifelse(fakedat$load=="hard" & fakedat$nountype=="occ",-1,
ifelse(fakedat$load=="hard" & fakedat$nountype=="name",1,0))
fakedat$rc_easy<-ifelse(fakedat$load=="easy" & fakedat$rctype=="subj",-1,
ifelse(fakedat$load=="easy" & fakedat$rctype=="obj",1,0))
fakedat$rc_hard<-ifelse(fakedat$load=="hard" & fakedat$rctype=="subj",-1,
ifelse(fakedat$load=="hard" & fakedat$rctype=="obj",1,0))
## subject random effects:
u<-mvrnorm(n=length(unique(fakedat$subj)),
mu=c(rep(0,8)),Sigma=Sigma_u)
## item random effects
w<-mvrnorm(n=length(unique(fakedat$item)),
mu=c(rep(0,8)),Sigma=Sigma_w)
## generate data row by row:
N<-dim(fakedat)[1]
rt<-rep(NA,N)
for(i in 1:N){
rt[i] <- rlnorm(1,beta[1] +
u[fakedat[i,]$subj,1] +
w[fakedat[i,]$item,1] +
(beta[2]+u[fakedat[i,]$subj,2]+
w[fakedat[i,]$item,2])*fakedat$ld[i]+
(beta[3]+u[fakedat[i,]$subj,3]+
w[fakedat[i,]$item,3])*fakedat$nn_easy[i]+
(beta[4]+u[fakedat[i,]$subj,4]+
w[fakedat[i,]$item,4])*fakedat$nn_hard[i]+
(beta[5]+u[fakedat[i,]$subj,5]+
w[fakedat[i,]$item,5])*fakedat$rc_easy[i]+
(beta[6]+u[fakedat[i,]$subj,6]+
w[fakedat[i,]$item,6])*fakedat$rc_hard[i]+
(beta[7]+u[fakedat[i,]$subj,7]+
w[fakedat[i,]$item,7])*(fakedat$nn_easy[i]:fakedat$rc_easy[i])+
(beta[8]+u[fakedat[i,]$subj,8]+
w[fakedat[i,]$item,8])*(fakedat$nn_hard[i]:fakedat$rc_hard[i]),
sigma_e)
}
fakedat$rt<-rt
fakedat$subj<-factor(fakedat$subj)
fakedat$item<-factor(fakedat$item)
fakedat
}
In order to estimate all parameters, we will have to fit a Bayesian model.
library(brms)
priors <- c(
prior(normal(0,10), class = Intercept),
prior(normal(0,1), class = b),
prior(normal(0, 1), class = sd),
prior(normal(0, 1), class = sigma)
)
m_brms<-brm(RT~ 1+ld + nn_easy + rc_easy + nn_hard + rc_hard +
nn_easy:rc_easy + nn_hard:rc_hard+
(1+ld + nn_easy + rc_easy + nn_hard + rc_hard +
nn_easy:rc_easy + nn_hard:rc_hard|subj)+
(1+ld + nn_easy + rc_easy + nn_hard + rc_hard +
nn_easy:rc_easy + nn_hard:rc_hard|item),
family=lognormal(),
prior=priors,
chains=4,
iter = 2000,
warmup =1000,
control = list(adapt_delta = 0.99,
max_treedepth=15),
df_fedorenko06)
## save model to avoid recomputation:
#save(m_brms,file="m_brms.rda")
Next, we extract the means of each of the parameters: 8 fixed effects, one standard deviation, 16 random intercepts and slopes by subject and item, and then 56 (gulp) correlations between subject intercepts and slopes and item intercepts and slopes.
load("m_brms.rda")
post<-brms::posterior_summary(m_brms)[1:81,1]
beta_names<-names(beta)
itemsd<-post[9:16]
itemcor<-post[25:52]
subjsd<-post[17:24]
subjcor<-post[53:80]
sigma_e<-post[81]
Next, assemble the correlation matrices. This would normally be very tedious because the off-diagonals have to be filled with the correlations. Luckily, R has a function for this that makes it really easy.
diagvals<-rep(1,8)
subjcorm <- matrix(NA, ncol = length(diagvals),
nrow = length(diagvals))
subjcorm[upper.tri(subjcorm)] <- subjcor
## fill up lower triangular matrix:
t_subjcorm<-t(subjcorm)
t_subjcorm[upper.tri(t_subjcorm)] <- subjcor
## symmetric matrix:
subjcorm<-t_subjcorm
diag(subjcorm)<-diagvals
## symmetric:
subjcorm==t(subjcorm)
## [,1] [,2] [,3] [,4] [,5] [,6] [,7] [,8]
## [1,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [2,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [3,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [4,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [5,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [6,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [7,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [8,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
Do the same for the items correlation matrix:
itemcorm <- matrix(NA, ncol = length(diagvals),
nrow = length(diagvals))
itemcorm[upper.tri(itemcorm)] <- itemcor
## fill up lower triangular matrix:
t_itemcorm<-t(itemcorm)
t_itemcorm[upper.tri(t_itemcorm)] <- itemcor
## symmetric matrix:
itemcorm<-t_itemcorm
diag(itemcorm)<-diagvals
## symmetric:
itemcorm==t(itemcorm)
## [,1] [,2] [,3] [,4] [,5] [,6] [,7] [,8]
## [1,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [2,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [3,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [4,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [5,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [6,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [7,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
## [8,] TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE TRUE
Next, assemble the variance-covariance matrices for subjects and items’ random effects:
Sigma_u<-SIN::sdcor2cov(stddev=subjsd,
corr=subjcorm)
Sigma_w<-SIN::sdcor2cov(stddev=itemsd,
corr=itemcorm)
In this simulation, we will fit two models, one with the full data, and one with the subsetted data. Then, we will check how often the SE associated with the critical interaction is larger in the full vs the subsetted model.
I computed some other summary statistics too (like power for the interaction), but looking at SEs summarizes my main point.
nsim<-100
betaintsefull<-betaintsesubset<-sigmafull<-sigmasubset<-tvalsfull<-tvalssubset<-rep(NA,nsim)
for(i in 1:nsim){
print(paste("iteration",i,sep=" "))
fakedat<-gen_fake_lnorm2x2x2(ncond=8,
nitem=32,
nsubj=48,
beta=beta,
Sigma_u=Sigma_u, # subject vcov matrix
Sigma_w=Sigma_w, # item vcov matrix
sigma_e=sigma_e)
mfull<-lmer(log(rt)~ld + nn_easy+rc_easy + nn_hard+rc_hard + nn_easy:rc_easy + nn_hard:rc_hard +
(1+ld + nn_easy+rc_easy + nn_hard+rc_hard + nn_easy:rc_easy + nn_hard:rc_hard ||subj) +
(1+ld + nn_easy+rc_easy + nn_hard+rc_hard + nn_easy:rc_easy + nn_hard:rc_hard||item),fakedat,
control=lmerControl(optimizer="nloptwrap", calc.derivs = FALSE))
sigmafull[i]<-sigma(mfull)
tvalsfull[i]<-summary(mfull)$coefficients[8,3]
betaintsefull[i]<-summary(mfull)$coefficients[8,2]
## subset the data:
fakehard<-subset(fakedat,load=="hard")
fakehard$noun<-ifelse(fakehard$nountype=="occ",1,-1)
fakehard$rc<-ifelse(fakehard$rctype=="obj",1,-1)
msubset<-lmer(log(rt)~noun+rc+noun:rc+(1+noun+rc+noun:rc||subj) + (1+noun+rc+noun:rc||item),fakehard,
control=lmerControl(optimizer="nloptwrap", calc.derivs = FALSE))
sigmasubset[i]<-sigma(msubset)
tvalssubset[i]<-summary(msubset)$coefficients[4,3]
betaintsesubset[i]<-summary(msubset)$coefficients[4,2]
}
save(betaintsefull,file="betaintsefull.rda")
save(betaintsesubset,file="betaintsesubset.rda")
The proportion of times that the SE of the interaction in the full model is larger than in the subsetted model:
load("betaintsefull.rda")
load("betaintsesubset.rda")
mean(betaintsefull>betaintsesubset)
## [1] 0.44
This means that the interaction has an overly enthusiastic SE for the critical interaction, when we subset the data. That’s not a good thing.
PS If there are any mistakes in the code above, please let me know and I will correct them.